Journal of Universal Language
Sejong University Language Research Institue
Article

Phonological Readjustment and Multimodular Interaction: Evidence from Kirundi Language Games

Jeanine Ntihirageza1
1Northeastern Illinois University

Copyright ⓒ 2016, Sejong University Language Research Institue. This is an Open-Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Published Online: Jan 01, 2017

Abstract

This paper investigates language games (also called ludlings [Laycock 1969, 1972, Bagemihl 1988a, 1995]) as linguistic tools used to unveil and understand phonological phenomena such as tone, vowel length and voice dissimilation. Although this study is comparative in nature, the focus is on novel data produced by Kirundi speakers. Three types of affixation language game data are analyzed. The goal of this investigation is twofold. First I show that the only length and tone that matter are the ones that come floating with the affixed segmental material. Second, this paper discusses the issue of source language, i.e., where in the phonology of the language the game rules apply. Following Bagemihl’s modular model (1988, 1995), couched in lexical phonology, I demonstrate that the source language of the language games belongs to different modules depending on the nature of the inserted material. I also show that one language game can pertain to different modules depending on its characteristics. Thus while the voice dissimilation rule is a level 2 morphological rule in Kirundi, the language game data indicate that this rule applies in both the lexical and post-lexical phonology as pointed out in (Goldsmith & Sabimana 1989, Goldsmith 1992). All in all, this study examines what insights into the regular phonology of languages can be obtained through an analysis of the language game data.

Keywords: language games; Kirundi; vowel length; Bantu; tone; voice dissimilation

References

1.

Alidou, O. 1997. A Phonological Study of Language Games in Six Languages of Niger. Ph.D. Dissertation. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University.

2.

Bagemihl, B. 1988a. Alternate Phonologies and Morphologies. Ph.D. Dissertation. British Columbia: University of British Columbia.

3.

Bagemihl, B. 1988b. The Morphology and Phonology of Katajjait (Inuit Throat Games). CJL 33, 1-58.

4.

Bagemihl, B. 1995. Language Games and Related Areas. In J. Goldsmith (ed.), The Handbook of Phonological Theory 697-712. Cambridge: Blackwell.

5.

Churma, D. 1985. Arguments from External Evidence in Phonology. New York: Garland Press.

6.

Clements, G. 1981. An Outline of Luganda Syllable Structure. SAL Supplement 8, 12-16.

7.

Clements, G & J. Goldsmith. 1984. Autosegmental Studies in Bantu Tone. Dordrecht: Foris.

8.

Cole, J. 1987. Planar Phonology and Morphology. Ph.D. Dissertation. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

9.

Cowan, N. & A. Lewis. 1990. Speakers' Access to the Phonological Structure of the Syllable in Word Games. CLS 26. 2, 44-59.

10.

Goldsmith, J. 1992. Note on the Genealogy of Research Traditions in Modern Phonology. Linguistics 28, 149-163.

11.

Goldsmith, J. & G. Clements. 1984. Autosegmental Studies in Bantu Tone: Introduction. In G. Clements & J. Goldsmith (eds.), Autosegmental Studies in Bantu Tone 1-17. Dordrecht: Foris.

12.

Goldsmith, J. & S. Firmard. 1989. The Kirundi Verb. In F. Jouannet (ed.), Modèles en tonologie. Paris: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique.

13.

Hayes, B. 1995. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

14.

Hombert, J. 1986. Word Games: Some Implications for Analysis of Tone and Other Phonological Constructs. In J. Ohala, & J. Jaeger (eds.), Experimental Phonology 175-186. New York: Academic Press.

15.

Idsardi, W. 1992. The Computation of Prosody. Ph.D. Dissertation. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

16.

Laycock, D. 1969. Sublanguages in Buin: Play, Poetry, and Preservation. Pacific Linguistics A 22, 1-23.

17.

Laycock, D. 1972. Towards a Typology of Play-languages, or Ludlings. Linguistic Communications 6, 61-113.

18.

Lehiste, I. 1985. An Estonian Word Game and the Phonematic Status of Long Vowels. Linguistic Inquiry 16, 490-492.

19.

McCarthy, J. & S. Alan. 1986. Prosodic Morphology. MS, Cambridge, MA: University of Massachusetts & Brandeis University.

20.

McCarthy, J. & S. Alan. 1990. Foot and Word in Prosodic Morphology: The Arabic Broken Plurals. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8, 209-282.

21.

Mohanan, K. 1986. The Theory of Lexical Phonology. Dordrecht: Reidel. Pulleyblank, D. 1986. Tone in Lexical Phonology. Dordrecht: Reidel.

22.

Selkirk, E. 1990. A Two-root Theory of Length. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers 14, 123-171.

23.

Sherzer, J. 1976. Play Languages: Implications for Sociolinguistics. In B. Kirshenblatt-Gimblett (ed.), Speech Play: Research and Resources for the Study of Linguistic Creativity 19-36. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

24.

Sherzer, J. 1982. Play Languages: With a Note on Ritual Languages. In L. Obler & M. Lise (eds.), Exceptional Language and Linguistics 175-199. New York: Academic Press.

25.

Yip, M. 1994. Isolated Uses of Prosodic Categories. In J. Cole & C. Kisseberth (eds.), Perspectives in Phonology 293-311. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.